Index page        Home page © Copyright2000  

CHAPTER 7: AUTISM AND RELATED CONDITIONS

HUGH MORGAN

INTRODUCTION

Autism  is one of a group of conditions called Pervasive Developmental Disorders, which usually manifest themselves in early childhood and persist into adulthood causing  long term disability, for which at present there is no “cure”. The most well known of these disorders are autism and Asperger’s syndrome. In recent years, more has become known about the factors involved in the causation of these disorders. Increased knowledge and experience  has led practitioners to develop improvements in to the approaches used to support individuals with these conditions and so improve  their quality of life.  This chapter will provide a “broad brush” overview of autism, describing how autism is  recognised  and responded to. Causation and diagnosis will be discussed, as will the needs of people with autism, and the services necessary to meet their needs.

A historical  perspective  is useful to understand the initial description and development of the concepts of autism and related conditions.

UNDERSTANDING AND DESCRIBING AUTISM

In  America in 1943, an Austrian psychiatrist, Leo Kanner, reported a group of children who seemed to share a pattern of behaviour which he christened 'early infantile autism'. The core behaviours he observed included the need for sameness in the everyday environment, repetitive (and often bizarre) routines, and a lack of emotional contact with others.  Kanner's observations were not entirely new, for it is  clear from literature throughout  history that people with autism lived before the condition was formally described. Kanner, however, identified the condition through meticulous description of affected children. He subsequently reported that the children in his study were characterised by 'autistic  aloneness'  and  an obsessional  desire for sameness (Kanner and  Eisenberg,  1956). Kanner felt that these characteristics were  significant, critical, features occurring during the first 30 months of life. Kanner used the term “ autistic “to reflect  the isolation from socially acceptable behaviour  apparent  within the children he had studied.

A year after  Kanner, Hans Asperger published a paper in German describing the findings of his  study of a group of children and adolescents with the condition that has come to bear his name. In 1971 Van Krevelin  compared the two disorders, and others subsequently published on the subject (e.g. Wing, 1981). There  were many similarities with  Kanner's  subjects, but also significant differences.  Asperger noted that the parents he had  interviewed had not recognised that their children had difficulties until they were at least three years old and, in several cases, not until they started  school. Unlike the children described by Kanner, those reported by Asperger had an adequate spoken vocabulary, with other abnormalities such as an obsessional interest in factual  matters (for example, one displayed an expert knowledge  of rail timetables). Like the children reported by Kanner, they had great difficulty in situations requiring two-way social interaction and communication. Essentially, they did not seem to be interested in, or to understand, the responses of other people.  “Autistic” was also the  term used  by Asperger to describe his group. He called this condition “Autistic Psychopathy “. This is now called Asperger’s syndrome, especially in Europe.

Neither Kanner nor Asperger were aware of each others’ research and on the face of it, the fact that they both used the term autistic to describe these children remarkable. However, this is not the case as the term had first been coined by an earlier psychiatrist Eugene Bleuler in 1911 to describe the narrowing of relationships to people and to the outside world and withdrawal into the person’s own self in schizophrenia. The term had been derived from the Greek word autos meaning “self “.Indeed, autism used to be considered a psychotic illness and the term childhood schizophrenia has been applied to it in the past. Today, the term autism is applied only to the condition which has its beginnings in childhood and pervades the person’s entire being ( Frith, 1989 ).

For many years the focus of practitioners and parents in the field of autism has been directed  by the descriptions  of  autism pioneered by Leo Kanner and Hans Asperger.  However, in  recent  years the work of authors such as Christopher  Gillberg  in Sweden  and Lorna Wing  in the UK,  has led researchers and other to conclude that autism and Asperger’s syndrome may be two variants of the same group of impairments. Wing and Gould in 1979 came up with a core triad of impairments which are accepted by professionals all over the world. They are : 1) qualitative impairment of reciprocal social interactions, 2) qualitative impairment in verbal and non-verbal communication and imaginative activity and 3) a markedly restricted repertoire of activities and interests. Autism and Asperger’s syndrome are now considered to be disorders of development, beginning in childhood but persisting into adulthood. As the impairments impact on most aspects of the individual’s life, the term Pervasive Developmental Disorders has been used to describe these conditions.

The two classificatory systems used all over the world, i.e. the International Classification of Diseases the 10th revision ( WHO ) and the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual IV (APA) both use the triad of impairments in the diagnosis of autism. Asperger’s syndrome describes people with similar impairments but with out the delayed or grossly abnormal speech typical of children and adults with autism.

How common is autism ?

The   incidence of autism   reflects awareness of  the syndrome by  practitioners and  parents. Over time, with increasing  familiarity and knowledge, the  number of individuals referred for assessment has increased. Before the establishment of the triad of impairments the prevalence was considered to be between 4 to 6 per 10,000 population. But Wing and Gould in 1979 found that while 21 per 10,000  showed the triad, only 4.9 met all the criteria for autism. Gillberg (1993 ) found autistic traits in 35 per 10,000 children who did not meet the diagnostic criteria for either Autism or Asperger’s syndrome. Epidemiological studies thus provide evidence for an Autistic Spectrum of disorders ranging from classical Kannerian autism to autistic traits.

Studies by Gillberg  and  his colleagues in Sweden led Wing (1996) to conclude that the incidence of  autistic spectrum  disorders  may be at least 6 per 1000 of the population (58 for  every 10,000 children).  This figure is significantly higher than the figures used by     the National Autistic Society in the UK in the 1980s as the basis for service planning.  The Society based estimates of the number of people with autism on a prevalence of  4 per 10,000 of the population.

This raises an important question: is the apparent increase in the number of people with autistic disorders purely  the result of increasing  awareness, combined with  the recognition of a number of conditions falling within the autistic  spectrum, or has there been a real increase in prevalence?  A true increase may, for example, be the result of genetic abnormalities produced by pollution (such as hydrocarbon-induced chromosomal breakage) or other effects of environmental toxins. No evidence has so far been found to support the hypothesis that autistic disorders are truly becoming more common.  

In their study in the inner London Borough of Camberwell in 1979, Wing and Gould identified children who did not necessarily fall into the category of Kannerian autism, but displayed profound difficulties in social interaction and communication. They were able to identify three groupings  based on the quality of their interaction: the aloof, the passive, and the active but odd. To this they added a fourth, the over formal, stilted subgroup (1996). In the original study they also found an association between IQ and the severity of social interactions. 

There is a highly significant association between autism and learning disability ( Olsson, Steffenburg and Gillberg, 1988).About 70% of people with autism have a learning disability and the prevalence of autism increases proportionally as the IQ decreases. Diagnosis also becomes more difficult as there is a considerable overlap between the features of both conditions. Asperger’s syndrome is not usually associated with learning disability although many clinicians have patients with mild learning disability and an autistic disorder closely resembling that described by Asperger.

Autistic spectrum disorders are often associated with other disorders which result from genetic or chromosomal abnormalities. Two such disorders can be used to illustrate the presence of autistic like patterns syndromes other than autism, and also to show how these behaviours can be differentiated from autistic spectrum disorders.

Fragile X syndrome, like autism is more common in men. Key features include hyper sensitivity to sensory stimuli such as sound and touch ,stereotyped behaviours and dislike of social contact with others. However, this avoidance of social contact is due to social anxiety and introversion rather than a failure to understand social meaning that is present in people with autistic spectrum disorders.

In Rett’s syndrome, another pervasive developmental disorder which affects only women, early normal development is followed partial or complete loss of speech and skills in locomotion and use of hands between 7 and 24 months of age. Behaviours similar to those seen in autism such as repetitive hand movements and lack of creative play together with impaired language development, severe learning disability and increasingly severe physical difficulties leads to social isolation. However, with appropriate management, the autistic like isolation becomes less pronounced.      

In recent years, speech and language therapists have promoted the concept of a communication disorder known as Semantic Pragmatic Disorder( SPD ), originally defined by Rapin and Allen (1983). This condition has been described as having similar deficits to those often described in Asperger’s syndrome and  includes difficulties in listening, talking, understanding language and how others think and feel, a lack of creative play and fine motor difficulties. There is often an exceptional memory for minor detail. There is a debate as to whether semantic pragmatic disorder should be seen as a separate entity or is simply a description of some of the characteristics of autistic spectrum disorders. Many practitioners believe that protagonists of the condition are failing to take into account all the deficits present when making their assessment, focusing selectively on certain aspects of behaviour and communication.   

How are autistic spectrum disorders caused ?

There are many theories on the causation of these disorders which can be considered under the headings of psychological theories and biological theories.

Psychological theories

Hobson postulated that the primary impairment was an inability to engage emotionally with others so that the child with autism does not receive the necessary social experiences to develop the cognitive structures for understanding (Hobson,1995).

Baron-Cohen and colleagues hold the primary impairment to be a cognitive one. Their theory of mind is based on the idea that the child with autism fails to develop the understanding that people have minds and mental states and that mental states relate to behaviour ( Baron-Cohen,1993) . 

Tager-Flusberg (1993) holds that the impairment is one of failure to develop an understanding of the nature of language as communication. Children with autism do not develop an understanding that communication and language exist for the exchange of information. 

Biological theories

There  is  increasing evidence that autism results from organically caused brain dysfunction. Studies have shown severe brain dysfunction in people with autism and coexisting syndromes such as tuberose sclerosis and phenylketonuria and medical conditions known to cause central nervous system pathology. It seems possible that multiple biological aetiologies may cause the syndrome of autism acting through a final common pathway which is as yet unknown.

Research involving the study of twin pairs has demonstrated a genetic  predisposition to autistic disorders: if one twin has autism, the likelihood of the other twin having autism is far higher for monozygotic (identical) twins than for dizygotic (non-identical) twins (Folstein and Rutter, 1977). Autism is far more common in boys than in girls (Lotter 1966; Lord and Schopler, 1987). Autism is  fifty times  more frequent in the  siblings of  people with autism  (Smalley  et  al 1988).Environmental  factors acting during the early stages of development (i.e. during  pregnancy and early life) are also thought to play a role.

Post-mortem studies on people with autism have shown little evidence of gross pathology in their brains. Computer assisted tomography and magnetic resonance imaging have shown abnormalities if the cerebral cortex, cerebellum and the ventricles in the brain. However, these abnormalities have not been consistent. Functional imaging such as positron emission tomography(PET) and single photon emission tomography (SPECT) again have not demonstrated any consistent abnormality. Perhaps the abnormality is too subtle to be picked up by the investigative techniques currently available.

Abnormalities have also been described in the brain chemistry . Platelet serotonin has been shown to be elevated in 30% of people with autism. Other abnormalities include low levels of dopa hydroxylase, elevated plasma noradrenaline and urinary homovanillic acid levels, elevated opiod levels in the cerebrospinal fluid with reduced plasma endorphins and abnormalities in the hypothalamic-pituitary axis which regulates the endocrine glands such as the thyroid. But a specific defect leading to the autistic syndrome remains to be found.      

Autistic disorders therefore seem likely to be the result of brain dysfunction occurring at a particular stage in development as no adult with “acquired autism” has been convincingly described. Some people, especially males, seem to be predisposed to this dysfunction, which may be mediated by a number of different biological processes.

DIAGNOSING AUTISM

The autistic child’s difficulties in the areas of communication, social interaction and  imagination and the restricted repertoire of activities are usually noticed by  parents, although they may not appreciate their significance. The abnormalities are sometimes first noted by practitioners such as teachers,  social workers, nursery staff or health visitors. The child's general practitioner will usually  be asked to make a referral to the local child health, child psychiatry or learning disability psychiatry service. There is no definitive diagnostic “test” for autistic spectrum disorders, although various assessment schedules are available to help structure information-gathering and for research purposes.

The key to correct diagnosis is a detailed developmental history from the child’s parents or any other informant who can describe the child’s behavioural and emotional state at different ages accurately. It would also be important to get information about family members who may also have learning disabilities and language problems. Diagnostic schedules or other standardised assessments may also be used to assist in making a diagnosis. Baron-Cohen (1992 ) developed a Checklist for Autism in Toddlers ( CHAT ) to assist in early diagnosis. Childhood Autism Rating Scale is a 15 item scale considered to be a valid and reliable tool for detection of autism in children and adolescents ( Schopler et al, 1986). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule( Lord et al 1989 ) and Autism Diagnostic Interview based on ICD-10 criteria for autism are two other scales available. It is useful to carry out tests of intellectual and communication abilities in order to tailor services to the individual’s needs.

The person with autism should have a full physical examination including a neuropsychiatric assessment and that of hearing and vision. Screening of urine and blood for metabolic disorders and chromosomal abnormalities can be useful. Electroencephalogram may be useful if there is associated epilepsy or a progressive neurological disorder is suspected. Other investigations such as magnetic resonance imaging need be carried out only if there is a clear indication.

SERVICES FOR PEOPLE WITH AUTISM

A  considerable  body of literature has evolved over the past 20 years regarding the needs of people with autistic disorders, and the most appropriate service responses. Accounts from researchers, practitioners, parents and the autobiographical writings of adults with autism such as Jim Sinclair, Donna Williams, and Temple Grandin, demonstrate that people with autism do not fit easily  into mainstream or so-called 'normalised' services.  The recurring theme in this literature is the need for individual support systems, based on an understanding of the basic impairments associated with autism, and reflecting the individual's need for predictability.

THERAPEUTIC APPROACHES

Pharmacological

People with autism will receive drugs for common physical ailments such as infections, and will need information about beneficial and possible adverse effects, presented in a way that takes account of communicatory and social difficulties. Prescribers will need to be aware of any physical disorder thought to be responsible for the autistic disorder (e.g. tuberose sclerosis) and of any co-existing disorder that may be exacerbated by drug treatment.

Medications of many types have also been used to treat psychiatric and behavioural disorders associated with autism. A distinction must be drawn between the treatment of specific psychiatric disorder in a person with autism, and the use of psychoactive drugs to treat behavioural abnormalities or components of the autistic triad of impairments.

People with autism are known to have variety of psychiatric disorders such as manic depressive illness. There may also be high levels of arousal and anxiety.  Antidepressants and lithium has been used successfully to manage cyclical mood disorders and propranolol to reduce arousal. It is important to remember that people with autism are likely to have idiosyncratic responses to medications of all kinds especially if they have an associated severe learning disability.

However, the efficacy of drugs in the treatment of behavioural abnormalities such as aggressive behaviours or severe self-injury is not well established. There have been several recent studies and reviews of the use of psychoactive medications  in the field of autistic-spectrum disorders. Howlin   et   al (1987)  suggested that there  is  only limited evidence that drug treatments for autism are effective, and little evidence for any effect on the autistic disorder itself. Clarke (1996) cautions that further research  is required before  firm  conclusions can be drawn regarding the effectiveness of drugs in the treatment of autistic spectrum disorders, although some recent research involving neuropeptide compounds and drugs affecting serotonergic systems shows some promise.  The   pharmacist  Shattock (1995) raised some general  concerns on  the  use  of medication to treat patterns  of  behaviour  often associated  with  autistic spectrum disorders.  He  believes that there may be physiological abnormalities associated with autism that make the prediction of treatment effects difficult, and individual differences (e.g. related to underlying causative conditions) that result in variations in response. The appropriate dose of a particular compound may be difficult to determine for similar reasons

Behavioural approaches

Behaviour modification is based on careful observation and recording of behaviour (rather than subjective interpretation) and the application of psychological techniques to promote desired behaviours and extinguish behaviours causing problems for the person or others. Behavioural approaches are useful for teaching new skills and highlighting specific environmental factors acting as triggers which may then be amenable to change. But  changes in patterns of behaviour learned in one setting are  rarely transferred  to another (“generalisation”). The implication is that any behavioural intervention should be in the individual’s normal environment.  

Educational approaches

During the 1950s and 1960s autism was often treated by  psychotherapeutic  techniques,  which  were founded on the premise that autism was an emotional, rather than cognitive, disorder, and  parents were seen to be at fault in this process. The psychotherapeutic  approach is still  adhered to in a few  countries, most notably in parts of southern Europe.    However, Rutter and Bartak (1973) demonstrated that an  educational approach, i.e. by applying structured and  consistent teaching within educational  settings, was far more  effective. Educational  approaches focused upon  helping the child or adult to understand and predict the world  in which they live have gained considerable impetus in recent  years, particularly in Northern and Western Europe, North America and Australasia.  

Achieving understanding

The most successful therapeutic strategies for people with autism are based on an understanding of the nature of autistic spectrum disorders. In the UK, the concepts  developed in the work of pioneers, like Sybil Elgar (who founded  the first school for children with autism in  the  world),  can be most clearly seen in the work of  present day  specialists working with children and adults with autistic spectrum disorders. The need to bring order and direction into the chaotic and confusing world of the individual with autism has featured in the literature available from many countries, including the enormously influential TEACCH programme in the USA.

Sign-posting

A  technique that is increasingly used in the  field  of  autistic spectrum  disorders  is  that of sign-posting.  In sign-posting, it is the task of practitioners to answer the questions that  the person with autism will have, in a manner that allows information to be gained and retained. Signposts encourage individuals to predict, with confidence, their daily activities.  Practitioners need to find methods of sign-posting which reflect a cognitive style centred on predictability and regularity.

It is also important to remember that, when establishing therapeutic environments for  people with autism,  practitioners should be aware of their own behaviour. This includes their use of verbal and non-verbal language (e.g. a calm  and reassuring tone of voice), and the clarity of the message given. The person with autism is likely to need answers to questions such as: Where should I be ? What do I have to do? When do I do it? How long will it last? Who do I do it with? The answers to these questions will have implications for the organisation of the environments in which the individuals live, work, study, and spend their leisure time.

A key task for practitioners is to help people with autistic spectrum disorders learn to manage time. Duration is an abstract concept, and "wait until next week" conveys little to many people with autism. The management of time can often be achieved  by scheduling,  by task organisation and by using daily timetables. However, for many people with autism, reading timetables, whether written or pictorial, will  be difficult. In these situations it may  be helpful to begin by  putting two activities in sequence through the use of pictures. There must be an end in sight;  without the certainty of a finishing point, anxiety-related behaviours may occur.

There are other alternative or augmentative methods of communication, including the use of rote learning, bliss symbols and sign-languages (Wilbur, 1985). The key to effective use is always a concrete  relationship between the sign or other aid and the meaning  behind the gesture used. Makaton, which is often used in services to adults with autistic spectrum disorders, and which is a derivative of the British Sign language for the  deaf, seldom makes use of signs which truly look like the meanings they represent. It is, therefore, often of limited use for non-verbal individuals with autistic disorders.

ORGANISATIONAL RESPONSES

Autism  cuts across all geographical boundaries and racial and ethnic groupings. The following similarities can be found:

·         the  experience of autism is the same for the  individuals and their families in any country

·         in the vast majority of countries, services have been started by the initiatives of parents and a few key professionals

·         services to children with autism are almost always developed many years before services for adults are considered

·         no country provides a full range of services capable of meeting the individual needs of children or adults with autistic  spectrum disorders  (Morgan, 1996)

The most appropriate service for someone with an autistic disorder will be determined largely by the capacity of the individual to learn the necessary social “rules”. The range of need to be found within the spectrum of autism is enormous. It would be inappropriate to  be prescriptive about one set of living circumstances that would be suitable for all people with autism. Whilst the autistic triad of impairments will be universally present, the behaviour of the individual will be profoundly influenced by other aspects of their personality, their level of global cognitive functioning, the presence of other disorders impacting on function such as dyslexia, dysphasia or dyspraxia, neuropsychiatric disorders such as manic depressive illness and epilepsy and by any co-existing sensory impairment. An  individual's unique personality, maturational changes, altered life circumstances, and the effects of the environment (including the  physical environment in which they live), will impact on the quality of support that will be required. For children, this is likely to mean identifying an appropriate school from a range of mainstream and specialist  provision, with the decision based upon an in-depth assessment of  need. In addition to state and voluntary sector schools for children with autistic  spectrum  disorders in the UK, there  are 'for-profit' organisations which provide  educational services to this section of the community.

For  parents, there is possibility of tapping into the mutual support available through the local  autistic  society, which may well have parent-led, support groups for parents. In the West Midlands, for example, there are 24 such self-help groups operating under the auspices of the West Midlands Autistic Society.

For  adults with autism, specialist services have, like many services for children, been instigated by parents. These services are sometimes  based in residential units managed by the National  Autistic Society,  but most are operated by local societies who have identified  the need for a locally-based service. Other types of provision include services developed by individual local authorities, and those offered  by private (i.e.  “for profit”)  organisations.   Traditionally, units offering residential services to adults have also included specialist day services, provided on site. Increasingly, specialist day services are being provided off-site, and fine examples of day services being established in mainstream colleges of further education are beginning to emerge in several parts of the country (Morgan, Edwards and Mason, 1996). Specialist health services for people with autism and additional health needs are discussed in chapter 10.

 The critical features determining  the success of these programmes appear to be the  knowledge and experience of autistic spectrum disorders held by both  support and teaching staff, and pivotal in this process will be an on-going staff training programme combined with excellent communication exchange.  It is clear that on its own, "normal" practice is simply not sufficient. There is a need for practitioners working in the field of autism to gain special skills and knowledge that differ from the theory and practice employed when working with non-autistic learning-disabled people. A programme based on reinforcing appropriate behaviour with socially based rewards, for example, is often applicable to non-autistic learning disabled people, but may be catastrophic for someone with autism. Initiatives such as the Educautisme programme in Europe and the UK based one year professionally  qualifying courses in autism operated by the University of Birmingham, are beginning to provide the type of autism-specific training required

 The National Autistic Society and the network of affiliated local societies have, since  1992, operated a quality  assurance programme - the NAS Network Autism Accreditation Programme. This provides the best guide yet that  parents, practitioners, and individuals themselves can have regarding the ability of a service to provide a truly autism-specific response to the individuals living or working within that service. Teams are selected from personnel with particular knowledge and experience, to visit a service in order to undertake an in-depth assessment of its capacity to respond specifically to the needs of people with autistic disorders.

USEFUL CONTACTS

The   Accreditation Centre for Autism Specific Services. 236   Henleaze  Road, Bristol BS9 4NG. (For information regarding the Accreditation programme).

National  Autistic  Society, 276 Willesden Lane, London,  NW2  5RB. (For  information  regarding the National Society  and  network  of Local Autistic Societies - there is likely to be one near you!)

The Course Co-ordinator, Autism, School of Education, University of Birmingham,  B15  2TT. (For  information  regarding  professionally  qualifying courses in autism) 

REFERENCES

American  Psychiatric  Association (APA). (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical  Manual of Mental Disorders (fourth edition) (DSM  IV). Washington DC. APA.

Baron-Cohen, S. ( 1993 ) From attention-goal psychology to belief-desire psychology: the development of a theory of mind and its dysfunction. In Understanding other Minds : Perspectives from Autism ( eds. S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg & D. J. Cohen ). Oxford : Oxford University Press.

Baron-Cohen, S. , Allen, J. , & Gillberg, C. ( 1992) Can Autism be detected at 18months? British Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 839-843.

Clarke, D. J. (1996). Psychiatric and behavioural problems and pharmacological  treatments.  In S.H. Morgan. Adults with Autism: A Guide to Theory and Practice.  Cambridge: Cambridge  University Press. 

Frith, U. (1989) Autism: Explaining the Enigma. Oxford : Blackwell.

Folstein,  S. and Rutter, M. (1977).  Infantile autism: A genetic study of 21 twin pairs.  Journal of Child Psychology and  Psychiatry, 18,  297-321.

Gillberg, C. (1990).  Autism and pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 99-119.

Hobson, R. ( 1995 ) Autism and the Development of Mind. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Howlin,  P.,  Rutter,  M., Bergin,M.,  Hemsley,K.,  Hersor,L.,  and Yule,W.  (1987). Treatment of Autistic Children. Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.

Jordan, R.R. and Powell, S.D. (1996).  In S.H. Morgan. Adults with Autism: A Guide to Theory and Practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 

Kanner, L. and Eisenberg, L. (1956). Early infantile autism  1943-1955.  American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 26, 55-65.

Lord, C., Rutter, M., Goode, S., et al ( 1989 ) Autism diagnostic information schedule: a standardised observation of communicative and social behaviour. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 185-212.

Lord,  C. and  Schopler, E. (1987).  Neurological  implications  of sex differences in autism.  In E. Schopler and G.B. Mesibov (Eds.).  Neurological Issues in Autism.  New York: Plenum Press.

Lotter,  V. (1966).  Epidemiology of autistic conditions  in  young children: prevalence.  Social Psychiatry, 1, 124-137

Morgan.  S.H.  (1996). Adults with Autism: A Guide to Theory and Practice.  Cambridge:  Cambridge University Press.

·        Morgan S.H., Edwards, G., and Mason, L. (1996).

·          ? INCORRECT CITATION?

*Adults with Autism: A Guide to Theory and Practice.  Cambridge.  Cambridge University Press.

Olsson,  I.,  Steffenburg,  S. and Gillberg,  1989). Epilepsy in autism and autistic-like  conditions - a population  based  study.  Archives of Neurology, 45, 666-668.

Rapin, L and Allen, D. (1983).  Developmental Language Disorders: Nosologic  consideration.   In U. Kirk (ed.)  Neuropsychology  of Language, Reading and Spelling.  New York: Academic Press.

Rutter,  M.  and  Bartak, L. (1973).  Follow-up and findings and implications for services.  Journal of Child Psychology  and  Psychiatry, 14, 241-270.

Schopler, E., Reichler, R.J. & Renner, B. R. (1986 ) The Childhood Autism Rating Scale. New York : Irvington.

Shattock, P. (1995).  In P. Shattock and T. Burrows (eds).  Promoting Physical and Material Well-being.  Module 2, Unit 3.   Distance Education  Course  in Autism (Adults).  Birmingham,  University  of Birmingham, School of Education.

Smalley,  S.L., Asarnow, R.F., and  Spence, A. (1988).  Autism  and genetics: A decade of research.  Archives of General  Psychiatry, 45, 953-961.

Tager-Flusberg, H. (1993) What language reveals about the understanding of minds in children with autism. In Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Autism ( eds. S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen ). Oxford : Oxford University Press.

Wilbur,  R.B.(1985). Sign language and autism.  In  E.Schopler  and G.B. Mesibov (eds.).  Communication Problems in Autism.  New  York. Plenum Press.

Wing, L. (1996).  The Autistic Spectrum: A guide for parents  and professionals.  London. Constable and Company.

Wing,  L., and  Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments of social interaction and associated abnormalities in children: epidemiology and classification. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 9, 11-29.

World  Health Organisation (WHO). (1992). ICD-10 Classification  of Mental  and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva. WHO.

Top of Page © Copyright2000